Theiling Online    Sitemap    Conlang Mailing List HQ   

Re: Sound Change Susceptibility

From:Isidora Zamora <isidora@...>
Date:Wednesday, November 5, 2003, 17:57
At 11:27 AM 11/4/03 +0100, you wrote:
>I was looking at a chart of reflexes of PIE and Proto-Semitic consonants the >other day, and noted that while, say, the ancestral velar stops are mangled >wantonly in many daughter languages, *m and *n are perserved in every language >listed (in initial position at least). > >Now, is this just a quirk of these particular families, or do different sound >have differing "intrinsic" probabilities for changing?
Andreas, I have *no* actual data to back this up, but I am going to venture a guess that certain sounds are going to be more prone to change, because certain sounds are inherently more difficult to produce. The interdental fricatives are objectively more difficult to pronounce than the alveolar fricatives. And that is something that I know know for a fact. I learned it from my phonology professor who had specifically studied this phenomenon and found a correlation between the age at which children learn to correctly pronounce these marked phonemes and the frequency with which the same phonemes were found in the world's languages. In addition to frequency, there is also a hierarchy: /s/ will be learned before /f/, which will be learned before /T/, if I am remembering correctly - that was over ten years ago, after all. /m/ is one of the first sounds that a baby can pronounce (thus accounting for the frequency of [ma] as a component in the word for 'mother' in a lot of unrelated languages. I would imagine that the sounds that are objectively more difficult to pronounce would tend to be more prone to change to something easier to pronounce, but there are plenty of examples of the opposite happening, so I could be completely wrong. Isidora

Replies

H. S. Teoh <hsteoh@...>
Isidora Zamora <isidora@...>