intuitive writing
From: | Moshe Blidstein <moblid@...> |
Date: | Tuesday, January 4, 2005, 13:03 |
Hello - I put down my thoughts on the subject of the present method of
writing and its problems. I would be very happy if you would take the time
to read it, and if it interests you enough and you have any suggestions or
further ideas, to send them to me at moblid@yahoo.com. Even if it doesnt
interest you, but you know someone that it might - please send it on to
him! Thank you.
Note - Many of the point below were made by earlier writers, and I could
footnote them to different sources, but I chose not to. Nevertheless, I
would appreciate if the readers will tell me if there are writers that talk
directly about these subjects, especially about answers to the problems.
Moshe Blidstein
Israel
I have been reading academic writing for quite a number of years, and have
come to the conclusion that the present way that we manipulate and pass
information - namely, writing and reading as we know it today - is most
problematic itself, and causes many secondary problems. Most of these
problems are taken for granted, and though there are many ways for
allegedly solving these problems, they dont get to the root of the matter
and are usually superficial. I will give my tentative ideas for helping to
solve these problems, ideas that I acknowledge need much more thinking
about and elaborating.
The problems
Text is highly inefficient from the eye-brain aspect. We read (usually)
much slower than we think. That is, our mind is idle most of the time we
read, waiting for us to get to the end of the paragraph so that it can at
last phrase the idea at hand to itself. This causes our mind to wander, and
we get diverted from what we are reading while we wait for our eyes. A word
doesnt convey much information, much less than a picture for example. Yet
the amount of time that it takes for the eye to process both isnt very
different. When we walk out of the door of our houses in the morning we are
confronted with a large amount of information, and we manage to internalize
it very fast. This is evident in driving, or any other task in which we
need to intake large amounts of visual information in a short amount of
time. But if the picture that we are seeing was described to us in writing,
we would need to read many pages till we would get the full picture.
People try to get around this problem by scanning - reading just part of
the words, and so more rapidly. Scanning sometimes works, but you always
lose some of the information and you never come out understanding too well
what youre reading.
I will start with the first, more technical, aspect of the problem.
Letters and words in all the languages I know are very uniform. They are
always the same color, on the same background; are of a similar shape, stay
in one line, and are usually the same font and style. Writing is always in
two dimensions, to suit paper, and because it works in lines it could
actually be called one-dimensional. None of these things are self-evident,
and could be done otherwise. The way they are now, letters and words convey
a small amount of information, and as we already said, our brain can handle
a much larger amount than is given to us in writing. Actually, present day
writing is a remnant of a culture based on parchment and manuscripts. The
invention of print changed almost only the technical aspects, but hardly
the medium itself. There is no logical reason to stay bounded to this form
of passing of information. The exact sciences have understood this point to
a certain extent. They use graphs, maps, logical symbol, etc. for the main
part of the discussion, and I think this is the step in the right
direction. In the humanities and social sciences graphs and maps are used
only to illustrate certain points, and are never the discussion itself,
because graphs are number-oriented. Trying to use graphs in the social
sciences and the humanities usually leads to confusion, because there is no
agreement on what the axes are, and so the axes chosen are usually
arbitrary or misleading, and the graph is many times quite un-
understandable. Charts of different kinds suffer from the same problems,
though they are usually clearer. But they always accompany the discussion;
never constitute a part of it.
Writing is, of course, talk-based. Even in languages that have different
systems for writing and talking (such as Arabic) the written version can be
vocalized, and the letters represent sounds. This may seem to make sense,
because we think in vocal language (some people are far gone enough tot
think in writing, too). But if you try to conjure your deeper or more
significant, or logical, thoughts, you will see that you usually state them
to yourself in graphic form, and sometimes in no form at all. Profound
ideas are many times just suddenly known, without a long tail of reasoning
stretching out in back of them. Thinking is organic as opposed to linear -
that is, we understand the whole idea at once and not point by point. I
believe, that the reason that we do think some thoughts vocally is just the
product of our use of language, and that changing the way we represent
ideas can also change the way we think, perhaps shifting our thought from
the present linear kind of reasoning that we usually attempt to a more
organic way of thinking.
When we read, we understand the text in jumps. We cant stop in the middle
of the paragraph - we have to get the whole idea before we can understand
it. But the present method of reading is step by step, in a linear fashion,
not representing at all the way that the thought or idea itself is
structured.
One of the results of this is that there is a big difference between what
the writer meant to say and what the reader reads (this may be very well in
a work of fiction, meant to be a work of art, but it wont do at all in
academic writing). The method of writing causes the idea to get lost in
translation, because the thought of the writer does not proceed in the same
orderly fashion that the text does (even if he is thinking vocally), but
rather jumps. The text does not parallel the readers thought either, for
the same reason; so now there are three divergent versions - the writer,
the text, and the reader, the reader being twice removed.
We can contrast the thinking represented by writing to the thinking that is
represented in a map of any kind. When you look at a map, you first see the
whole picture at once. The eyes first go the main contours, the thick lines
and the big letters. After that, you start looking in to the part of the
map that interests you, going deeper and deeper into it. The thinking that
the map portrays is non-linear - it lets the eye jump about at will, in a
many-dimensional matrix. There are many colors, lines, letters, but this
plethora isnt chaotic to the eye because the map uses a language that
people have learnt to use. The amount and depth of information that a
geographical map conveys is unparalleled by any text. A map can be looked
at from many angles, different people can take from it what they need, and
yet the information will be exact for every one of them (incidentally, that
is why a map costs so much more than the same amount of paper covered by
text).
Text, on the other hand, looks at a first glance like a block of black
squiggly lines and at second glance it looks that way too. The only way to
get at the information is to read it, word by word. There are no
intermediate levels, no dimensions.
A different problem, that many artificial logical languages (such as
lobjan) have tried to fix, is the indeterminacy of words, that is, there
are many ways to say the same things and there are many words that mean a
lot of things. This becomes more pronounced as we rise from words to
sentences to paragraphs - even if we know exactly what a word means, it is
much harder to say the same about a paragraph. This is also a technical
problem (whether in talking to computers or to humans), but it is more than
that - I can read two authors that say the same thing, and I wont know it,
because they said it in very different words. Even two people talking to
each other can pass a good hours worth of conversation before they
understand that they agree. Or, I may be reading two authors saying
opposite things, and I wont know that either.
Present methods dont show you the logical steps inside the text. The idea
that is portrayed always has a certain hierarchy of meaning comprising it.
In any argument, there is the logical core, the examples, assumptions,
problem and criticism, etc. But all of these look in the text exactly the
same, and actually the only way that a reader can know what is probably
most important, or where the core of the matter is, is by the volume of
writing - whatever the author says again and again is probably the most
important. The logical core is sometimes hidden from the reader by the
other parts, leading him to evaluate wrongly the text he is reading.
Sometimes there is no logic at all behind the argument, or the logic is
faulty, but since the logic is never stressed anyway, we arent expecting
it to, and content ourselves with the fun of the reading. Readers are
persuaded much more by the artistry of the writing than by the ideas
themselves, because the writing isnt suited to show the ideas as they are,
but rather the writers are usually showing how well they write. Writing
should be an instrument of thought, not a way of obscuring it. There is
some kind of logical division inside the text - punctuation marks. But
these usually follow speech, and they just serve to show the need for this
kind of logical division. We never went further than this, because we were
afraid to change the main graphic configuration of the text, other than
inserting little marks.
Different writers never use the same language, even when they all write in
English. Each one uses his own private one. The result is that when I want
to compare the ideas of two authors to each other, I have two translate
both of them into a third language, my own (this is a further duplication
of the reader-twice-removed problem that we saw before). Usually, if nobody
points it out to me I will never even have noticed that the two authors are
related to each other or disagree, because their language is so different,
because they use different terms, or come from different topics. In the
last decades this problem is becoming more acute, because the scientific
universe has expanded so much that it is much harder to talk from one side
of it to the other - say, from psychology to international relations. This
is a well recognized problem, but there arent any forthcoming solutions,
because nobody can learn every topic, and so each one is left to his own
language.
This brings us to a further problem. The number of texts is so great, that
even if you talk the language of the topic you wont be able to know which
text you should read to retrieve the information you are interested in,
unless you have already read all the books in the topic and have
photographic memory. Indexing and library methods arent anywhere near
giving enough information, and even so the indexes themselves are getting
much too big to handle, leading to bibliographies of bibliographies of
subjects. Each book is put into some very wide category, leaving no way for
the reader to find what he is looking for. This is understandable - no
librarian can read all the books he gets every day, let alone understand
them. For example, I was looking on the internet for the problem I am
writing about now, but I had no idea how it is called, so I couldnt look
for it. There are certain categories that it may fall into, but they are
much too wide to be any help. The internet search engines can only look for
key-words, maybe the more complicated algorithms that are starting to be
used can do a bit more, but to really know whats written and if its worth
anything, you have to read it. The result of all this is that we are
swimming an ocean of information, and arent able to find what we need.
Academic writing has to be made self-indexing in some way, because no one
else can contrive effective indexing other than the writer itself.
A further problem is that since the main language in academia is English,
everybody has to learn it before they can become something. But it isnt
enough to learn just normal talking English - they have to learn
sophisticated, complex academic English, because that is how the academia
writes. People that are less proficient in learning languages - and they
are many bright people that arent - have a very hard time because of this.
In short, there should be a way of representing ideas in a much more true,
exact, efficient, rational, multi-dimensional way. I want to accentuate
that I am not referring to talking language, or to fiction or even some non-
fiction writing, such as newspapers, fact-books, etc. I am referring mainly
to more professional books which convey abstract ideas and put forward
arguments that are supposed to be logical.
Another point. When we throw a ball, run, or talk, we are doing a very
complex action, usually putting to use many different muscles in a very
exact and complex way. This is true in anything we do without thinking
about it. Contrasted to these actions, things we do consciously and not out
of intuition are very poorly done, and many times dont work either. The
short amount of evolutionary time that our conscious and logical part of
the brain had didnt serve it too well, as opposed to the many millions of
years that our intuitions and instincts had. Were very new at using this
part of the brain, and dont do it so well. All big discoveries and ideas
seem to be made mostly be the intuitional part of the brain, and less by
the logical part. I certainly dont mean to say that the logical part of
the brain should be shut down. What I do mean is that logical thinking
should be made as close as possible to the other parts, so that we would be
able to understand an idea as adeptly as we catch a ball, with all the
minute and complex computation that this requires. This can be helped by
changing the method of representation of thoughts, making them more
graphic - more like maps, for example.
Before I try to give the beginning of what may be an answer to these
problems, I want to point out one attribute of our writing method that is
positive - its great flexibility and subtlety. Because words are so uniform
and unstructured, you can put them together in myriad ways and forms, to
produce whatever kind of text you want. Exactly because the form of the
writing is always similar, and there arent any rules pertaining to how to
write logically (except for syntax), the sky is the limit, and almost any
information can be passed in this way. But this is a mixed blessing. The
great freedom of the writer has become excessive. It is suited well for the
fiction author, the political or religious persuader. But it is not suited
for the academic writer, who tries to make a logical, understandable point,
because this freedom results in too much text, un-indexable, in which the
logic isnt apparent to the reader.
A possible approach to an answer
As the reader who got up to here may have understood, I think that the
answer lies in substituting the present text for a more complicated one.
This text will not be a whole new language - it will use heavily the usual
written language. But:
1. It will use many more marks, such as color, font, letter size,
modes etc. But you wont be able to just stick them in wherever you want
(present-day texts with a lot of marks are very confusing because each
writer uses them however he wants. If they will always signify the same
idea, they shouldnt be any more confusing than a topographical map after
you know how to read it).
2. Instituting about a hundred (if possible, less) marks, that will
represent words or ideas. The kind of words I have in mind are most of the
logical symbols: bigger - smaller - equals, inside (a group)
or outside, causing - caused, leading to, if and only
if, objective - subjective, opposed to - agreeing to parallel,
etc. etc. The idea is to have symbols for the logical aspects of the
discussion, not so as to make them shorter, but so that they will stand
out.
3. Place on the page - the idea is that someone looking at the page
will understand immediately where the main argument is, where the auxiliary
arguments are, in social sciences - theory as opposed to empirical data,
implications and elaborations, sources and bibliography, etc. This idea is
already present in footnotes - we know that the lesser arguments and the
sources will come at the end of the book or on the bottom of the page. But
why should sources and lesser arguments be put together? They have nothing
in common. Each component will have its special place; also inside the
different components, the argument itself, for example, can be arranged
graphically, color coded, or something similar, to show its further
components
This kind of writing will draw the reader straight one to the main argument
if he wishes, or to the former theories that he has to take for granted, or
to the empirical evidence. Furthermore (and maybe more important), it will
force the writer to include all of these elements. A writer wont be able
to put in an argument without real support, because it will be evident
right away that that is what he did. Or, it will force him to show what the
main argument is - he cant just write up a jumble of examples without any
clear outcome, and so on. This will clarify thought considerably, raise the
standards of ideas and writing, and will make the idea much more
understandable. Even when you dont understand, it will be much easier to
know what exactly you dont understand.
A good version of page-placement will enable you to look at the page in at
least two different ways - from afar, in which you will be able to see the
general lay of the argument, and from close up, where you will be able to
see the more exact ideas.
4. These techniques may be used to solve the indexing problem as well.
The page placement, colors etc. will be used to show the importance of
different parts of the idea. A computer will be able to read these codes,
and so it will know the level of importance. When you go searching in your
computer, you wont be searching for the title or for the small number of
subjects the text is usually indexed under; you will be looking for the
whole text, but in different levels, which the writer himself instituted.
Thus, the computer will be able to read the text itself, even though it
doesnt understand it, and give you a good approximation of whats written
there.
5. A very important point is that all these symbols and signs will be
the same for all writers, and so it will be much easier to compare and
decide between different opinions. It will also enable better communication
between different disciplines and fields, because they will be talking a
similar language, if not exactly the same one. To give an example from
history - Roman cities were always built on the same patter - the bathouse
was always in the same approximate place, and so was the courthouse, the
agora, and so on. This may pose aesthetic problems, but a roman would have
felt at home in any city, and would know right away where to go to
6. I believe that there are further hierarchies of meaning, apart from
importance, which can be shown in graphics, if we only decide on it - of
subject (going deeper or drawing out of a subject), empirical/theoretical,
and others.
The main idea is that that there will be some kind of correlation between
the form of the writing and what it actually says. We can do this by using
many dimensions - in color and placement, and that way you also use your
brain-time more efficiently, and make text closer to the physical world,
which is what our minds are best at doing. Style will not persuade the
reader, but the content itself.
The basic unit of thought is larger than a word, it is an idea; thinking
that x is similar or not to y; that x leads to y, in different kinds of
ways; that x should be y; etc. The way we convey these ideas should also be
larger than the word, showing the relationships inside the idea in a
graphic form. The symbol should correlate to the symbolized.
I think that these suggestions are less utopian than other language
suggestions, which usually turn the whole language over. These ideas can be
used at first alongside the usual text, and if they really will be better
and easier to use - they will eventually substitute them.
The humanities and social sciences seem to be in a near stand-still as
compared to the exact sciences. We know much more, but it doesnt seem as
if we understand much more than 30 years ago. This may seem like a short
period to be talking about, but other sciences have gotten so far in this
amount of time! I believe that one of the reasons is, that the methods we
use are exactly the same - thinking and writing. We cant use computers
(almost), we cant use graphs and charts. They all need exact information
to work. The breakthrough has to come in the methodological side of things -
like methods of writing that will enable to us use a different kind of a
graphic system.
P.S.
I was reminded of an alleged syndrome that has come up in the last few
years, called the power-point syndrome. As a result of using power-point
slide shows to organize and discuss their ideas, corporate workers have
become more stupid (or so they say), because they have to show all their
thoughts in short points and in flowcharts. They cant think more elaborate
or complicated thoughts, just point by point ideas. This may be true, but
doesnt have much to do with the thoughts detailed above. I think that you
can show very elaborate ideas in these techniques. Just because you can
write easily that This caused that (with no further complications)
doesnt mean that anybody will agree with you. On the contrary, it will
much easier to see that you are wrong. The difference between power-point
and this (I need a name for it
), is that here the flowcharts dont have
arrows that can mean anything, but rather each arrow will have a
different logical meaning, leading to a richer and more exact logical
apparatus. There is no reason the sophistication of the idea will be any
less, it will just be much more evident what the sophistication is. In the
same way, graphs and equations didnt make mathematicians more stupid than
they were before - they gave them better tools to understand each other
than they would have had if they had just talked. Flowcharts show the need
of graphic, intuitional representation, but they are so arbitrary in their
design, that they can only be useful for shallow and un-exact thought.
Reply