Theiling Online    Sitemap    Conlang Mailing List HQ   

Re: LANGUAGE LAWS

From:Mathias M. Lassailly <lassailly@...>
Date:Wednesday, October 21, 1998, 19:20
Tommie wrote :

Nik Taylor wrote:
> > > I agree with you that only purely > > polysynthetic is restricted to stone age peoples, but I see no reason to > > think that polysynthetic is "less sloppy" than other types, and there's > > absolutely no evidence that I can see to say that it was the *only* type > > spoken before agriculture. Polysynthetic would probably have been more > > common, and perhaps the relative ratios of the other types may have been > > different, who knows? But on what evidence do you say that the human > > mind "may be predisposed to creating purely polysynthetic structures"? > > When we are tiny children learning to speak our native language, nobody teaches us what > its morphemes mean: We figure out their meanings by ourselves, from the various contexts > within which each morpheme appears. And a morpheme typically has a rich constellation > of meaning, so that what a typical morpheme means in one context isn't quite the same as > what it means in other contexts. (I'll grant that some morphemes -- such as pronouns > and numerals -- may have invariant meanings, but nearly all other common morphemes > don't.) > > The reason why a typical morpheme means something different in one context than it means > in other contexts, is that different portions of its "rich constellation of meaning" > come into play in different contexts. > > What purely polysynthetic structures do -- and other types of grammatical structures > don't do -- is provide a strictly limited number of contexts in which any morpheme can > appear. So, if the human mind is "predisposed to creating purely polysynthetic > structures", that means it is predisposed to limiting the variety of meanings that a > morpheme can have. > > That tendancy would logically go hand-in-hand with the tendancy to give morphemes "rich > constellations of meaning" -- since giving them rich constellations of meaning > automatically leads to them meaning different things in different contexts, so that the > only way to limit proliferation of a morpheme's meanings is by limiting how many > contexts it can appear in! > > Of course, any type of linguistic structure keeps a morpheme from appearing in some > contexts, and hence limits proliferation of a morpheme's meanings to some degree. > > So I have to agree with you: There's no reason to believe that *all* Stone Age languages > were purely polysynthetic until the advent of trade languages. > > -- Tommie > >
I strongly agree with you. Also, these languages often refer to one specific context by means of a locution made of several morphemes. For instance, 'to give' would be referred to as 'hand...give' and 'cow' as 'animal-cow'. I think that Europeans often underestimate these 'classifiers' as 'redundant', whereas I do believe they are an inherent part of the concept evoked. It's not a question of compounding but of limiting and identifying the concept meant. Maybe 'grammar' originate from some of these parts of words having gained mandatory syntactic role ? Mathias ----- See the original message at http://www.egroups.com/list/conlang/?start=17630 -- Free e-mail group hosting at http://www.eGroups.com/