Re: Construct case and genitive pronouns
From: | Patrick Littell <puchitao@...> |
Date: | Monday, August 29, 2005, 6:18 |
On 8/27/05, tomhchappell <tomhchappell@...> wrote:
>
>
> In the phrase "Carsten his Birthday"
> "Carsten" is the Dependent, "Birthday" is the Head;
> "his" is a Floating Marker, attached neither to the Head nor to the
> Dependent. If the order is typically (Dependent, Marker, Head) this
> might diachronically evolve into a Genitive Dependent Marker; if the
> order is typically (Head, Marker, Dependent) it might diachronically
> evolve into a Construct-State Head Marker.
So far on this list we've mostly talked about morphologically marked
construct state vs. a zero-marked absolutive state, and how it might come
about. (Pretty much as above.) There's two other interesting sources for
statey-ness that we might also mention.
The first is the underspecification of some feature on the head, with an
assumption that the head will "inherit" the feature from its dependent. I
don't know how idafa originally came about, but this a reasonable guess.
That's underspecification of definiteness; we played around with the idea of
underspecification of number earlier. What else might be possible? That'd be
interesting to consider, but I'm way too tired at the moment. State as
underspecification of *case*? That'd be pretty strange. I'll have to think
about that later.
The other is the emergence of a morphologically marked absolutive state as
an indication that a usually inalienably-owned possession is being talked
about without regard to any general posessor. For example, in some languages
it's rare to talk about body parts or familial relations without marking
them as mine or yours or his or hers. (It's rare to talk about an arm that
doesn't belong to anyone, and about a mother that isn't anyone's mother.) In
Smith's Manual of Spoken Tzeltal (available on the internet; search for it),
he talks about the difficulty in eliciting the unpossessed word for "hand".
The usual response for a hand that isn't mine or yours or his... "our hand".
So we might have the usual, unmarked form of a noun being the construct
state -- the possessed one -- while there's a separate, less common version
for the very marked occasion of talking about hands and mothers "in
general", such as "Mothers are for comfort; fathers are for play."
This pattern would be a bit strange, however, for *all* nouns. It's weird to
talk about a hand that doesn't or didn't belong to anyone, but to whom
belong clouds, centuries, and colors? I don't believe ANADEW, but I wouldn't
find it strange to find a language with two different state-marking
paradigms depending on the ownership-tendencies of the noun in question. One
in which nouns of the first type (body parts, relatives, personality traits,
etc.) are in construct state unless specifically marked with an absolutive
affix, and in which nouns of the second type (colors, wild animals,
earthquakes, etc.) are in absolutive state unless specifically marked with a
construct affix. (And, if we're being naturalistic, one in which everything
in between -- houses, horses, hatchets, etc. -- are unpredictably assigned
to one or the other!)
Hope this doesn't overwhelm you, Carsten, but I find it to be interesting
food for thought. It's a birthday present of random information!
-- Pat
P.S. While you're on a head-marking kick don't forget about inflected
prepositions!
Reply