Theiling Online    Sitemap    Conlang Mailing List HQ   

Re: Evidence for Nostratic? (was Re: Proto-Uralic?)

From:Jörg Rhiemeier <joerg_rhiemeier@...>
Date:Friday, July 4, 2003, 14:56
Rob Haden <magwich78@...> writes:

> On Thu, 3 Jul 2003 10:47:05 +0200, =?iso-8859-1?Q?J=F6rg=20Rhiemeier?= > <joerg_rhiemeier@...> wrote: > > >I am not an expert on Kartvelian, but I found some information on this > >in Gamkrelidze's and Ivanov's book, _Indo-European and the Indo-Europeans_. > > > >First, the phonology. PIE, if one accepts glottalic theory (which I do), > >had a three-way distinction between voiceless, voiced and ejective stops, > >as does Kartvelian (and other Caucasian languages); and apparently, > >Kartvelian has an ablaut system similar to that of IE. > > It seems to me that ejective stops can also be called aspirated stops.
No. Ejective stops are *not* aspirated. (See Thomas R. Wier's explanation.)
> I've seen some e/o variations in Georgian morphology, particularly in > affixes, but nothing as seemingly paradigmatic as in PIE. Can you give any > examples of Kartvelian Ablaut?
I know too little about Kartvelian to give an example, but Thomas has already done so.
> Also, regarding Ablaut, my hypothesis is that most of the contrasts in > vowel quality (e vs. o) were originally contrasts in tone. It is well- > noted that vowels did not undergo alternation in the presence of > a "laryngeal" (?, h, x, etc.).
They did, but *e became *a next to *h2 and *o next to *h3.
> In certain places, I think that contrasts > in vowel quality were caused by other (unknown?) factors.
Yes. There were probably several factors affecting vowel quality.
> >The common points in syntax are SOV order (a frequent pattern that > >proves nothing and is also shared by Proto-Uralic) and active-stative > >argument marking. That is, intransitive subjects are marked like > >transitive subjects if they are agents (as in 'The man runs'), > >but like transitive objects if they are not (as in 'The stone falls'). > >Typically, only animate nouns can take agent marking. > > SOV is, apparently, a very ancient (and likely original) word order. In my > opinion, the first great split within Nostratic occurred when the southern > speakers (> Afro-Asiatic) adopted VSO instead of SOV.
Not all Afro-Asiatic languages are VSO: most Cushitic langauges are SOV, and Chadic languages are SVO. According to some scholars, the three branches for which VSO order is typical (Semitic, Berber and Egyptian) form a distinct subgroup. Anyway, a shift in word order wasn't the factor that led to the split between A-A and the rest of Nostratic (assuming the relationship is real, which might not be the case).
> I've seen people try to prove that Proto-Uralic also had stative verbs, but > I simply see no evidence for it except for the Hungarian indefinite > conjugation. Then again, why would the PU stative paradigm disappear > everywhere but Hungarian?
If it occurs only in Hungarian (not even in Ob-Ugric?), then it is most likely a Hungarian innovation. Unless one can prove that Uralic is related to some other languages which show something that is demonstrably cognate to the Hungarian forms.
> >This pattern is partly preserved in Kartvelian languages, and traces > >of it can be found in IE (active vs. stative verb endings, avoidance > >of inanimate transitive subjects, syncretism of nominative and > >accusative in neuters (animates had an agentive case in *-s > >and an objective case in *-m, later to become nom. and acc. > >respectively, while inanimates had an unmarked objective case > >and no agentive case). > > The origin of the animate sigmatic nominative and the nominative-accusative > syncretism in inanimates is of great interest to me. My opinion is that > the former was originally an ergative marker,
Yes; or an agent marker (the difference is that the latter is also used with intransitive verbs with active semantics, such as "to run").
> which in turn came from the > genitive.
Possible.
> Syntactically/grammatically, this makes sense: the genitive case > is the case of origin, and only animate nouns can "originate action" (i.e., > perform an action). The problem lies in reconciling this with the current > reconstruction of PIE. As for the nominative-accusative syncretism, it is > obvious that inanimate nouns could never be grammatical agents, and thus > never had a true nominative in PIE.
Exactly.
> When the rules of grammar shifted in > later times, speakers used the most common case for inanimate nouns -- the > accusative -- as the nominative as well. Does this make sense?
Yes.
> However, > it is of interest that this only occurred with thematic inanimates; > athematic inanimates never had a marked accusative case. I think the most > likely reason for this is that the athematic inanimates were an older > class;
Yes, the thematic nouns are a rather late development.
> they were root nouns (which later disappeared/became thematized), > i/u-stems (relatively rare), and s-, r-, and n-stems (which appear to have > been rather productive at some point in PIE). Even after the original root > nouns were thematized, the remaining stems were still able to be > distinguished solely by their endings (except for s-stems in the nominative > singular, which I think became confused with thematic animates, e.g. > *nebhos, *nebhes- 'cloud' < **nebhes).
The distinction between s-stems and thematic stems survived quite well; they still were distinct classes in Classical Latin at least 3000 years after the breakup of PIE (e.g. corpus, gen. corporis < *corpos-is).
> >Why does this point to a substratum? It is a well-known fact that > >learners of a foreign language have the greatest difficulty with > >phonology (leading to accents) and syntax. To give one example: > >Native German speakers, when speaking English, often use > >the perfect in situations where a native speaker of English > >would use the simple past, but the perfect is used in German. > >(The German perfect has a wider range of meaning than the > >English perfect.) So when a population switches to another > >language, features of their old language tend to be carried over > >into the new language in the fields of phonology and syntax > >rather than morphology. > > There could very well have been one or more substrata in PIE; the problem > lies in finding it.
Yes. It remains a hypothesis.
> >Yes. And possibly, transitive verbs agreed with both arguments, > >as would be the typical pattern in an active-stative language. > >The thematic vowel might be the last remnant of a 3rd person object marker. > > > >Jörg. > > I have also considered this. However, there does not seem to be a clear > pattern whereby transitive verbs are always thematic, and intransitive ones > are always athematic.
You are right; there is no clear pattern. The development would be 3rd person object marker > transitive marker > stem-forming element. The stem-forming element was semantically empty and no longer associated with transitivity.
> For example, *gwhen- 'strike, slay, kill' is > athematic (*gwhenmi 'I kill') but transitive. In any case, however, there > is a discernable pattern in the e/o alternations of the thematic vowel: the > o alternation always occurs before a nasal. Whether this apparent sound- > change was conditional or not remains to be seen. I think that either > thematic verbs resulted from a 3rd person object marker, or that they were > simply vocalic verb stems (i.e., ending in a true vowel, from the > standpoint of PIE).
This is possible.
> I certainly think that the latter is the reason for > the contrast between thematic and athematic nouns.
Some scholars suspect an adjectival origin for thematic nouns, but that remains speculative. Jörg. ____________________________________________________________________________ Jetzt bei WEB.DE FreeMail anmelden = 1qm Regenwald schuetzen! Helfen Sie mit! Nutzen Sie den Serien-Testsieger. http://user.web.de/Regenwald