Theiling Online    Sitemap    Conlang Mailing List HQ   

Re: THEORY: Evolution of infixes/ablaut?

From:Tom Wier <artabanos@...>
Date:Sunday, March 19, 2000, 8:19
Nik Taylor wrote:

> Eric Christopherson wrote: > > I'm really fascinated by the idea but I > > can't figure out how the mind would allow a morpheme to be modified from > > the inside -- just seems like morphemes "should" be concrete, unbreakable > > elements to me. It's a bias in my conlanging instinct I guess :)
I'm by no means a morphologist, but I imagine that there are a number of ways for this to come about. Suppose, for example, that you have two forms ['aSum] and ['aSulum] (for "hand", say), where [-um] is a basic syntactic morpheme (say, nominative) and [-ul] is a pluralizer. That much is pretty basic, straightforward morphology: a root [aS-] with one or two suffixes, depending on the circumstance. Let's further assume that some basic phonological rule comes through that deletes all nuclear vowels syllables in word final position where accent is not immediately adjacent. In this case, that would only apply to the second [u] in ['aSulum] to produce ['aSulm]. Here we are presented with an irregularity: ['aSum] yet ['aSulm]. There are two ways a child learning this language can approach this: he can assume that the base form of the morphemes realized as [-um] or [-m] is underlyingly /-um/ and allow for that kind of allomorphy, or, if the pattern is persistent enough across a wide variety of lexemes, that the [-l-] is in reality a morpheme /-l-/ that is stuck right into the middle of what, to all other appearances, seems to be a homogenous suffix [-um]. Either way is possible. Now then, why don't we see more infixing in languages? Because we do see them more frequently than one might think -- it is extremely common in languages of Mexico and Central America (though there it is admittedly an areal feature). If the phenomenon is mostly absent from modern European languages, that could be owned up to a dearth of morphology in general, because to have that kind of complicated process shown above you need at least a certain level of morphological resources for it to work with.
>I'm not sure either, perhaps it's just spontaneous or something. After
> all, colloquial vulgar English uses infixes quite frequently, as in > abso-f***ing-lutely, or a line I read somewhere "Down in > Tumba-bloody-rumba shooting kanga-bloody-roos"
Frequently? Certainly not on a daily basis. Not for me or anyone I know, at least.
> Perhaps sometimes it's simple metathesis.
Metathesis is only extremely rarely a regular phenomenon. If a language makes *regular* use of infixation, then some other process will also have to be invoked; and the more processes you have to invoke to get your result, the flimsier the theory must be, by Occam's Razor.
> Another guess is that perhaps it started with only some words.
Well, all linguistic change begins like this. The question is: what are the processes driving that linguistic change? Reanalysis is, as you say, a good way for that to occur. =========================================== Tom Wier <artabanos@...> "Cogito ergo sum, sed credo ergo ero." ===========================================