Theiling Online    Sitemap    Conlang Mailing List HQ   

Re: Tensed Pronouns (was: Tagalog ...)

From:Tim Smith <timsmith@...>
Date:Sunday, March 7, 1999, 4:01
At 05:26 PM 3/3/99 GMT+0, And Rosta wrote:
>Tim Smith: >> In fact, one of my back-burner conlang projects is a future language that=
I
>> call neo-Anglic, which does precisely this -- except that I've made some >> fairly radical simplifications (like abolishing gender) which I justify=
by
>> saying that it's descended from an English-based creole rather than=
directly
>> from English. > >Do you have any info on neo-Anglic? Details of the lg itself and >about who speaks it where when? As I mentioned yesterday, my >equivalent, /brI-S/, has of late thrust itself onto the front-burner, >and I'm particularly keen to see an American counterpart so as to >assess possible inihibitory and exacerbatory effects it might have on >the evolution of British (=3D English+Welsh) English. >--And.
As a matter of fact, I do have a brief sketch of Neo-Anglic, to which I've just made some hasty revisions (hence my slowness in responding). As you can see, it's very unfinished: there are many important areas that it doesn't mention, such as questions, focus marking, relative clauses (or any kind of subordinate clauses, for that matter). But it should give you a rough idea of what I had in mind. I really have no idea where this language is spoken, or by whom, or how far in the future. I don't really know enough about language change to give a good estimate of the minimum time it would take for it to evolve from a "generic" English-based creole, or even how plausible it is. (The demonstrative system, in particular, is strictly whimsical, and probably can't be justified on any rational linguistic grounds.) I have some ideas about further changes that I might make in it, which I'll say something about in a follow-up post. Anyway, here it is. Warning: it's pretty long as posts to this list go -- about 20 kilobytes. Also, it contains some characters that aren't standard ASCII, so for those of you whose e-mail software has trouble with the ISO Latin-1 character set: =E1 =3D a with acute accent =E9 =3D e with acute accent =E8 =3D e with grave accent =ED =3D i with acute accent =F3 =3D o with acute accent =F2 =3D o with grave accent =FA =3D u with acute accent NEO-ANGLIC, VERSION 2 Overview Neo-Anglic is a future language descended from a mesolectal variety of an English-based creole. From the isolating grammar and fixed word order of the creole protolanguage, it has evolved into an agglutinative language (at least with respect to verbal morphology) with relatively free word order, as subject and object pronouns and auxiliary verbs became first cliticized and later fully bound to the verb stem as agreement markers and as tense and aspect markers, respectively. Phonology and Orthography All the letters of the Roman alphabet are used except C, and all have their lower-case IPA values except for the following: Consonants: {y} =3D [j] {j} =3D [Z] {x} =3D [S] {h} =3D [x] or [h] depending on dialect {q} =3D [N] Vowels: {e} =3D [e] in open unstressed syllables and in stressed syllables when marked with an acute accent, [E] in closed unstressed syllables and in stressed syllables when marked with a grave accent {o} =3D [o] in open unstressed syllables and in stressed syllables when marked with an acute accent [O] in closed unstressed syllables and in stressed syllables when marked with a grave accent [This vowel system, with seven phonemic vowels in stressed syllables but only five in unstressed syllables, is essentially the same as that of Italian.] Diphthongs: {ai} =3D [aI] {au} =3D [AU] {oi} =3D [OI] Stress is phonemic, and is marked by an acute or grave accent on the nuclear vowel of the stressed syllable. An acute accent is used with {a}, {i} or {u}, and either an acute or a grave accent with {e} or {o} (see above). These diacritics are also used with stressed monosyllables, to distinguish them from function words which are always unstressed, such as prepositions. Conversely, there are bisyllabic function words that have no stressed syllables, such as prepositions with unemphatic pronominal objects (see below under "Prepositions"). Nouns Nouns are not morphologically marked for case, number or definiteness. Case roles are indicated by prepositions and by subject and object agreement marking on the verb. Number and definiteness are generally recoverable from context, because a referential-indefinite noun phrase must be preceded by a quantifier (either a cardinal number or one of a small group of adjectives with meanings like "some", "several", "many", "a few", etc.); any unquantified NP must be either definite (in which case its referent, and hence its number, is already known to the listener) or non-referential (generic, in which case its number is irrelevant to the discourse). (In context, non-referential NPs are unlikely to be confused with definite ones, as evidenced by the use of the definite article with both in English, e.g., "the blue whale" meaning either a specific blue whale or blue whales in general.) Moreover, if the NP is either a subject or a definite direct object, its number will be marked by a pronominal affix on the verb. Thus, _m=E1n_ can mean either "the man", "the men", or "men" (in general), depending on the context, while _wan m=E1n_ means "a (specifc) man" and _pl=E8ni m=E1n_ means "some men". There is an optional proclitic plural marker _al_, which can be used with a definite NP if its number is felt to be ambiguous in context. This is a phrasal clitic, preceding the NP as a unit rather than the noun itself, e.g., _al m=E1n_ ("the men") or _al =F3l m=E1n_ ("the old men"), but not _*=F3l al m=E1n_. Adjectives Adjectives can either precede or follow their head nouns, depending on discourse-pragmatic factors. The default order is adjective-noun, but this may be reversed to emphasize the adjective; the noun-adjective order is mandatory when the adjective is under contrastive stress. Thus, _=F3l m=E1n_ =3D "the old man" (neutral), but _m=E1n =F3l_ =3D "the OLD man" (as opposed to the young man that we were also talking about). Adjectives, like nouns, are uninflected. Comparison is by means of the preposed particles _m=F2_ (more), _m=F3s_ (most), _l=E8s_ (less), _l=EDs_ (least). Adjectives as such are used only attributively, never predicatively. The equivalent of predicate adjectives are stative verbs formed by adding verbal affixes to adjective stems, e.g., _man id=E8d_ =3D "the man is dead". Personal Pronouns Independent Prefix Suffix 1st sg. m=ED a(y)- -(a)mi 1st pl. incl. =E1inai ana(y)- -(a)na excl. =E1s wi(y)- -(a)s 2nd sg. y=FA yu(w)- -(y)u 2nd pl. y=F2l yal- -(y)al 3rd sg. --- i(y)- -(i)m 3rd pl. --- de(y)- -(d)em The independent pronouns can occur in any position in which a noun can occur, but are normally used only for emphasis. The pronominal prefixes mark subject agreement on verbs. The pronominal suffixes mark object agreement on verbs, but only with definite direct objects; they are also used as unemphatic pronominal objects of prepositions. There are no independent personal pronouns for the third person; demonstratives are used instead (see below). Demonstratives Demonstrative adjectives Singular Plural this/these (near the speaker) d=EDs ald=EDs that/those (near the addressee) d=E8t ald=E8t that/those (far from both) di=E1da aldi=E1da Demonstrative pronouns ------ Animate ------ ----- Inanimate ----- Singular Plural Singular Plural d=EDsgai ald=EDsgai d=EDswan ald=EDswan d=E8tgai ald=E8tgai d=E8twan ald=E8twan di=E1dagai aldi=E1dagai di=E1dawan aldi=E1dawan Demonstrative adjectives, like other adjectives, normally precede their head nouns, but follow them when used with contrastive emphasis. Thus: _d=EDs m=E1n_ (this man), _ald=EDs m=E1n_ ("these men"), but _m=E1n d=EDs_ ("THIS man"), _m=E1n ald=EDs_ ("THESE men"). When a demonstrative and another adjective qualify the same noun and neither is contrastive, the demonstrative comes first: _d=EDs =F3l m=E1n_ ("this old man"). Note that the plural prefix _al-_, although formally identical to the optional noun-phrase plural clitic _al_, is NOT optional with demonstratives. Moreover, the use of a plural demonstrative in an NP precludes the use of _al_ as a phrasal clitic. (Thus *_al ald=EDs m=E1n_ and *_al m=E1n ald=EDs_ are ungrammatical.) Prepositions True to its creole origins, Neo-Anglic has only a very few prepositions. The most frequently used are: lo(q) to, at (general-purpose dative, allative and locative marker; also marks accusative for specific, animate direct objects [like Spanish _a_ or Hebrew _et_]). bla(q) of (genitive marker). auta(v) from, away from (ablative marker). bai with, by, by way of, through (instrumental, comitative and perlative marker) (A final consonant, shown above in parenthesis, occurs only when the following word begins with a vowel.) When the object of a preposition is a personal pronoun rather than a noun, the pronominal suffix may be used, e.g., _b=FAk blami_ ("my book"), _h=E1us blaqana_ ("our [exclusive] house"), _m=E1n idaqg=EDvim b=FAk loyal_ ("the man gave you [plural] the book").=20 Note that these "conjugated prepositions" have no stressed syllables. They are not used if the pronominal object of the preposition is a focus, a contrastive topic, or otherwise emphasized; in these situations the independent pronouns are used, e.g., _b=FAk bla m=ED_ ("MY book"). [Since there are no independent third-person personal pronouns, demonstrative pronouns are used instead, e.g., _b=FAk bla aldi=E1dagai_ ("THEIR book").] To express finer locational or directional distinctions than are allowed by the small number of available prepositions, a class of "local nouns" are used. A local noun is positioned between a preposition and its nominal object to clarify the meaning of the preposition, so that formally the local noun is the object of the preposition and the lexical noun stands in a genitive relation to the local noun. For example, _lo h=E1us_ =3D "at the house" or "to the house", but _loq =EDnsaid h=E1us_ =3D "in the house" or "into the house". [In formal and written registers, the latter would be _loq =EDnsaid bla h=E1us_, making the genitive relation between the local noun and the lexical noun explicit (literally "at/to the inside of the house").] The potential ambiguity between static and dynamic readings of prepositions such as _lo_ (that is, "at" vs. "to" and "in" vs. "into" in the examples above) is resolved in most contexts by the semantics of the verb. In general, telic verbs of motion require a dynamic interpretation, while atelic verbs of motion and all other verbs require a static one. (See below under "Verbs" for an explanation of the telic vs. atelic distinction.) Verbs The affirmative active verb has the following morpheme slots: SUBJECT-[TENSE/MODALITY]-[ASPECT1]-STEM-[ASPECT2]-[OBJECT]. Subject and object agreement: The subject and object agreement markers are the pronominal prefixes and suffixes, respectively (see above). All verbs except imperatives are marked for subject agreement, but only definite direct objects trigger object agreement. The following is a sample paradigm of an affirmative verb in the present tense (lexical stem _s=ED_ "see"): Subject Direct Object 1st sg. 1st sg. ---- 2nd sg. as=EDu 3rd sg. as=EDm 1st pl. excl. ---- 1st pl. incl. ---- 2nd pl. as=EDal 3rd pl. as=EDdem none or indef. as=ED 2nd sg. 1st sg. yus=EDmi 2nd sg. ----- 3rd sg. yus=EDm 1st pl. excl. yus=EDna 1st pl. incl. yus=EDas 2nd pl. ----- 3rd pl. yus=EDdem none or indef. yus=ED 3rd sg. 1st sg. is=EDmi 2nd sg. is=EDu 3rd sg. is=EDm 1st pl. excl. is=EDna 1st pl. incl. is=EDas 2nd pl. is=EDal 3rd pl. is=EDdem none or indef. is=ED 1st pl. excl. 1st sg. ------ 2nd sg. anas=EDu 3rd sg. anas=EDm 1st pl. excl. ------ 1st pl. incl. ------ 2nd pl. anas=EDal 3rd pl. anas=EDdem none or indef. anas=ED 1st pl. incl. 1st sg. ----- 2nd sg. wis=EDu 3rd sg. wis=EDm 1st pl. excl. ----- 1st pl. incl. ----- 2nd pl. wis=EDal 3rd pl. wis=EDdem none or indef. wis=ED 2nd pl. 1st sg. yals=EDmi 2nd sg. ------ 3rd sg. yals=EDm 1st pl. excl. yals=EDna 1st pl. incl. yals=EDas 2nd pl. ------ 3rd pl. yals=EDdem none or indef. yals=ED 3rd pl. 1st sg. des=EDmi 2nd sg. des=EDu 3rd sg. des=EDm 1st pl. excl. des=EDna 1st pl. incl. des=EDas 2nd pl. des=EDal 3rd pl. des=EDdem none or indef. des=ED Tense and aspect marking: There are two aspect markers: an imperfective suffix _-(i)n_, which goes immediately after the lexical stem and before the object suffix (if any), and a completive or perfect prefix=20 _-dan-_, which goes after any tense and/or modality prefixes and before any passive or inchoative prefix. The imperfective suffix is used only with non-stative verbs, since stative verbs are inherently imperfective. The perfect prefix has taken on the function of marking perfective past (preterite or aorist) as well as perfect (like, for example, the French "perfect" tense formed with _avoir_ plus the perfect participle). There are also two tense markers, both prefixes which immediately follow the subject prefix and precede any aspect prefixes: a past or anterior marker _-bin-_ and a future marker _-gon-_. The tense markers can combine with each other and with the aspect markers to form a wide variety of tense-aspect combinations. Rather than attempting to formulate clearly and concisely the rules governing the co-occurrence of the various tense and aspect markers, I will simply list the possible combinations of these morphemes with two verbs, one stative and one non-stative. In order to illustrate how the tense and aspect markers interact with subject and object agreement markers, both verbs in these examples have a first-person singular subject and a third-person singular direct object. Stative (_s=E1vi_, "know"): as=E1vim -- "I know it" (present) abins=E1vim -- "I knew it" (past) agons=E1vim -- "I will know it" (future) abingons=E1vim -- "I was going to know it" (post-past) abindans=E1vim -- "I had known it" (pluperfect) agondans=E1vim -- "I will have known it" (future perfect) abingondans=E1vim -- "I was going to have known it" (post-past perfect) Non-stative (_t=E9k_, "take"): Perfective: at=E9kim -- "I take it" (present) adant=E9kim -- "I took it" (preterite) or "I have taken it" (perfect) agont=E9kim -- "I will take it" (future) abingont=E9kim -- "I was going to take it" (post-past) abindant=E9kim -- "I had taken it" (pluperfect) agondant=E9kim -- "I will have taken it" (future perfect) abingondant=E9kim -- "I was going to have taken it" (post-past perfect) Imperfective: at=E9kinim -- "I am taking it" (present) abint=E9kinim -- "I was taking it" (past) agont=E9kinim -- "I will be taking it" (future) abingont=E9kinim -- "I was going to be taking it" (post- past) abindant=E9kinim -- "I had been taking it" (pluperfect) agondant=E9kinim -- "I will have been taking it" (future perfect) abingondant=E9kinim -- "I was going to have been taking it" (post-past perfect) Note: A nasal consonant at the end of a prefix assimilates to the point of articulation of the following consonant (if any), so that, for example, _abingont=E9kim_ is pronounced as if it were spelled _abiqgont=E9kim_. Passives and inchoatives: A transitive verb can be passivized by inserting the prefix=20 _-get-_ immediately before the lexical stem, after any tense, modality, or aspect prefixes, e.g., _m=E1n idangets=E8n lo s=EDdi_ ("the man was sent to the city"). In general, the passive is used only when the agent of a transitive verb is unknown or unspecified. Topicalizing the patient or focusing the agent (the other main uses of the passive in English) are usually accomplished simply by changing the word order, e.g., _lo m=E1n idans=EDm l=E9di_ ("the man was seen by the woman"). (_m=E1n idangets=ED bai l=E9di_ is grammatically correct but would be perceived by native speakers as sounding awkward.) The same prefix, used in the same way with a stative intransitive verb, forms an inchoative verb (a non-stative verb meaning to take on the state denoted by the original verb), e.g., _m=E1n idangetd=E8d_ ("the man died", i.e., became dead). Negation: The verb is negated by inserting the prefix _-no-_ immediately after the subject prefix (before any tense, modality or aspect prefixes) and placing the negative particle _nan_ after the verb (rather like the French _ne...pas_), e.g.: affirmative yus=EDm "you see it" negative yunos=EDm nan "you don't see it" Phrasal verbs: There is a class of verbs whose stems consist of two lexical roots, both of which can, in most cases, also stand as independent words. The first element is a verb and the second is an adverb or adjective which qualifies in some way the meaning of the first. Examples are _ran=F3va_ ("run over"), _mekd=E8d_ ("kill"). In verbs of this type, suffixes (if any) are inserted between the two lexical elements, rather than attached to the end. Thus: _tr=E1k idanranim=F3va lo m=E1n_ ("the truck ran over the man") not _*tr=E1k idanran=F3vam lo man_ Telic and atelic verbs: The telic vs. atelic distinction is related but not identical to the stative vs. dynamic distinction. A stative verb, as the name implies, denotes a state, while a dynamic verb denotes a process.=20 Telic verbs are a subset of dynamic verbs (and thus atelic verbs are a superset of stative verbs): a telic verb denotes a process which is in some sense goal-directed, that is, one which can in principle be completed, whether or not it actually is completed in a given situation. In other words, a telic verb denotes a process which is intrinsically bounded, whereas an atelic verb denotes either a state or a process which is potentially open- ended. =20 Most verbs of motion are telic in their basic form, but can be made atelic by the addition of the suffix _-raun_. With verbs of motion, telicity is what determines whether a preposition denoting a spatial relationship is interpreted statically (as a locative marker) or dynamically (as an allative marker). For example, _idanw=F2k loq =EDnsaid h=E1us_ means "he walked into the house", but _idanw=F2kraun loq =EDnsaid h=E1us_ means "he walked (around) in the house". Word Order The most common word order in transitive sentences is SVO, with the direct object immediately following the verb and oblique objects expressed by prepositional phrases following the direct object, e.g., _m=E1n idang=EDvim b=FAk lo l=E9di_ ("the man gave the book to the woman"). However, there is considerable freedom to vary this order based on pragmatic factors, since the subject and object agreement marking on the verb and the use of the preposition _lo_ as an accusative marker (when the direct object is animate and specific) are usually sufficient to disambiguate syntactic roles. There is a strong tendency toward verb-second order, especially in main clauses, but this tendency is mitigated by a strict rule that a referential-indefinite NP cannot precede the verb. Thus intransitive sentences with indefinite subjects are mandatorily VS, e.g., _idank=E1m wan m=E1n lo haus_ ("a man came to the house").=20 This rule also leads to many OSV sentences, e.g., _lo m=E1n idanranim=F3va wan tr=E1k_ ("the man was run over by a truck"). The general rule is that the topic precedes the verb and everything else follows it. This means that sentences without topics (such as existential sentences) are verb-initial. Existential sentences are formed with the impersonal verb _gat_, with invariant third-person singular subject marking, e.g., _igat t=FA m=E1n loq =EDnsaid h=E1us_ ("there are two men in the house"). Fossilized Expressions Greeting: =E1i informal aw=E1ya formal Leave-taking: s=EDa informal bis=EDnya somewhat formal abis=EDnya very formal ------------------------------------------------- Tim Smith timsmith@global2000.net The human mind is inherently fallible. It sees patterns where there is only random clustering, overestimates and underestimates odds depending on emotional need, ignores obvious facts that contradict already established conclusions. Hopes and fears become detailed memories. And absolutely correct conclusions are drawn from completely inadequate evidence. - Alexander Jablokov, _Deepdrive_ (Avon Books, 1998, p. 269)