Theiling Online    Sitemap    Conlang Mailing List HQ   

Re: Language superiority, improvement, etc.

From:Matt Pearson <mpearson@...>
Date:Wednesday, October 14, 1998, 17:45
Joshua Shinavier wrote:

>The argument that "all languages are equal because there is no way of provi=
ng
>one superior to another" is a confused one; if there is no way of >comparing the >worth of languages, then their "equality" is a meaningless concept.
Agreed!
>There *are*, however, properties of languages which *can* be compared quite >effectively, logic and complexity being two of the easiest to define a >means of >measurement for --
I'm not so sure about that. The last time this issue was discussed on Conlang, we tried to figure out some objective criteria for measuring relative logic and complexity, without much success (or at least, *I* was unconvinced). The only real quantifiable feature that people could come up with was relative number of morphologically irregular forms. The idea, I guess, is that morphologically irregular forms are harder to learn (both by native speakers and by second language learners), making a language that has more irregular forms more 'complex' and less 'logical' than a language with less irregular forms. However, morphological irregularity is such a marginal phenomenon in all languages that I question whether it can be taken as a serious measure of relative complexity or logic. All languages are overwhelmingly compositional, and I can't see that it makes much difference to GLOBAL complexity whether a language has, say, 12 irregular verbs or 42. Another thing to keep in mind is that, even with objective criteria to measure them, 'complexity' and 'logic' depend very much on your point of view, and on how well you understand the phenomenon you're examining. An interesting example that just recently came to my attention is the distribution of the Accusative and Partitive cases in Finnish. As many of you probably know, direct objects in Finnish can appear in either the Accusative or the Partitive case (or the Nominative case in imperatives and certain non-finite constructions): H=E4n luki kirjat he read-Pst book-Acc.Pl "He read the books" "He read some books" H=E4n luki kirjoja he read-Pst book-Part.Pl "He read some of the books" "He was reading books" Whether this is system is simple or complex depends on what your initial assumptions are. If you take the Accusative form to be the 'basic' one, and the Partitive form to be the 'marked' one (as most teachers of Finnish do), then the system turns out to be quite complex, insofar as you need to learn large number of rules in order to know when to use one form and when to use the other. For example: (1) The Partitive must be used in place of the Accusative in negative clauses (2) The Partitive must be used after prepositions (3) The Partitive is required with the subjects of resultative clauses (4) Use the Partitive instead of the Accusative with certain present tenses (5) Certain verbs select the Partitive in place of the Accusative in all tenses Et cetera et cetera. HOWEVER, if you assume that the Partitive is the 'basic' direct object form and the Accusative is the 'marked' one, then suddenly the system becomes extremely simple. You can basically state the distribution of the two cases with one rule: (1) Use the Accusative in place of the Partitive when the verb denotes a completed event in which the object is totally affected The point I'm trying to make is that whether a particular linguistic phenomenon seems random (and hence 'complex' and 'illogical') may depend on your analysis. One set of assumptions may lead you to conclude that there's no pattern to the data, while another set of assumptions may reveal that the data is actually highly structured and sensible - on its own terms. Matt. ------------------------------------ Matt Pearson mpearson@ucla.edu UCLA Linguistics Department 405 Hilgard Avenue Los Angeles, CA 90095-1543 ------------------------------------