Theiling Online    Sitemap    Conlang Mailing List HQ   

Re: A Self-segregating morphology (was: Guinea pigs invited)

From:Jim Henry <jimhenry1973@...>
Date:Friday, December 16, 2005, 16:39
On 12/16/05, Gary Shannon <fiziwig@...> wrote:

> How about this for a self-segregating morphology with > a non-flat sound: (self-segregating at the word > boundry, but not at the root+root boundry withi8n a > word.) > > First vowel determines the length of the word in > syllables. Remaining vowels can be anything.
This is similar to Jeff Prothero's "Plan B" and the projects discussed by Ray Brown and Jörg Rhiemeier in the thread "brz, or Plan B revisited" back in September -- except there, it was the initial consonant that determines the number of phonemes the listener is to expect in the word. .....
> To cover very long words, when the first and second > syllable both have the same vowel expect the word to > be double the normal length: > > "komotegulimaxusu", or "kakadaso".
Nice. I suspect this might be a little easier for the human brain to parse in real-time than the schemes Messrs Prothero, Brown and Rhiemeier came up with, but one would need to experiment to make sure.
> Vowels are irrelevant to the identity of the word, so > when words are compounded the first vowel is changed > to match the new word length: "galo" + "haki" = > "golohaki"; "ki" + "kedaso" = "kokedaso", OR > "kakadaso".
Hm... So if you have a word "kanu" you could not also have a word "kani"?
> With a few other rules about which vowels may follow > which consonants in which contexts it would not even > be necessary to write the vowels down at all. Thus
....
> With perhaps a few dozen "standard" vowel sequence > patterns, possibly choosen based on intial consonant, > the language could have a very melodic and varied > cadence. For example, suppose that two-syllable words > beginning in "k" use the pattern "au", four-syllable > words beginning with "d" always used the pattern > "oaiu" while four-syllable words beginning with "n" > always used the pattern "aaui". Thus "g dxtk kn ntpt" > could only possibly be "gi doxatiku kanu nataputi", > which is not at all flat and monotonous, yet remains > audibly self-segregating and completely determined > even without writing down the vowels.
Alternatively, you might (allowing free choice of vowels in morphemes after the first syllable) set a _default_ vowel to follow each consonant, which would save space while writing but still give you a lot more potential morphemes at any given length than your original scheme. So a word like /nataputi/ following the default vowel pattern could be written "ntpt" but you are still free to have morphemes like /natapote/ written "ntpote", etc. I'm not sure, but I suspect that self-segregation at the morpheme level is more important than self-segregation at the word level. Word boundaries are clear from spacing in writing, and usually clear from stress accent in speech (for the languages I'm most familiar with); but morpheme boundaries within compound words are more likely to be tricky, at least for students of a language if not for fluent speakers. I've had problems due to uncertainty about morpheme boundaries in learning Esperanto and Volapük and to a lesser extent Greek, but rarely if ever with word boundaries. -- Jim Henry http://www.pobox.com/~jimhenry/conlang.htm ...Mind the gmail Reply-to: field

Reply

Gary Shannon <fiziwig@...>