Theiling Online    Sitemap    Conlang Mailing List HQ   

Re: THEORY/YAEPT: Re: Terkunan: rules for deriving nouns, verbs, adjectives

From:T. A. McLeay <conlang@...>
Date:Friday, November 2, 2007, 17:08
On Wed, 31 Oct 2007 08:31:31 -0400, "Mark J. Reed" <markjreed@...>
said:
> Could you elaborate on the pronunciations of those pairs? Most of > them are perfect homophones IML. (The exception being finger/singer, > which don't even have the same vowel in the first syllable.)
What difference?
> I'm not sure about "madder" the color, since I've never heard that > word before, but I suspect it would sound the same as the "angrier" > version. Is there a parallel distinction between the herpetic and > arithmetic meanings of "adder" in those same dialects?
If "madder" is monomorphemic, it'd be [m&d@] for me; whereas if it's bimorphemic (mad+er) then it's [m&:d@]. Likewise "gladden". But before -d the distinction is only available in four adjectives (bad, glad, mad and sad), and so "adder" only has one pronunciation. (However, I've never heard the words "madder"=brown or "gladden"=iris before, but the above are the obvious spelling pronunciations.) But, most of the examples And lists below confuse me. "Gladden" seems to be the only example of a sound change being aware of morphological boundaries; the rest are created using the same simple rules before and after the sound change has ceased to become active. -- Tristan.
> > On 10/30/07, And Rosta <and.rosta@...> wrote: > > Dirk Elzinga, On 30/10/2007 18:33: > > > On 10/30/07, Jörg Rhiemeier <joerg_rhiemeier@...> wrote: > > >> The GMP *should not* distinguish between morphological endings and > > >> normal stem ends (where they are word-final), because real sound > > >> changes *do not make such distinctions*. You are trying to simulate > > >> something that *just doesn't happen* in natlangs. If a final -m > > >> goes away, for instance, it does so no matter what kind of morpheme > > >> it is part of. > > > > > > It is not true that sound changes do not take morphological boundaries > > > into account. Consider the following examples from a non-standard > > > variety of English: > > [...] > > > So it seems that morphological information is crucial to understanding > > > this change, and your statement that "sound changes don't care the > > > least of the morphological structure > > > of the word" is not true, or is at best overstated. > > > > Further examples from English (English being the language I know something > > about): > > > > Here are some minimal pairs in various accents of English. > > > > finger : singer [everywhere but NW England] > > > > madder (brown) : madder (more mad) > > gladden (iris) : gladden (make glad) [various places] > > > > pause : paws [SE England] > > > > hula : ruler [SE England] > > > > holy : holey [SE England] > > > > nose : knows [Leeds] > > > > pride : pried [Northumbria] > > > > brood : brewed [Scotland, Ireland] > > > > The second item in each pair contains a morpheme boundary & has a phonetic > > realization found only when a morpheme boundary is present. > > > > The notion that phonology is blind to morphological juncture is an erroneous > > (and nowadays obsolete?) piece of dogma. > > > > --And. > > > > > -- > Mark J. Reed <markjreed@...>

Replies

Mark J. Reed <markjreed@...>
And Rosta <and.rosta@...>